Skip to content

Early time-restricted feeding for the prevention of diabetes

More and more students find themselves staying up late in order to complete assignments, study for tests, make time for a job, or maintain a social life. In our previous blog, Clocks in College, we discussed the notion that many students experience circadian disruption without realizing the full implication that it has on their health. Most students understand on some level that this fluctuating schedule can have negative effects (they are tired, experience more anxiety, etc) but rarely do students realize that disruption of their circadian rhythm is increasing their risk for many metabolic diseases (see our blog: Biological Clocks). Even more surprising, is that most students have no idea how much their own study schedule could be impacting their ability to learn.

For students, the logic is simple: Less time sleeping means more time to cram for that exam. According to some studies, as many as 60% of students claim to pull-all nighters, and their reasons were twice as often academic as social (Thacher 2008). Research shows, however, that circadian disruption has significant detrimental effects on learning and recall, as shown by many animal studies. First of all, most animals learn best during their natural wakeful period. Cockroaches, rats, and mice—all nocturnal—are more successful in various memory tasks when they learn during the nighttime. (Decker 2007, Hauber 2001, Hoffmann 1992, respectively). Rats that were phase-shifted 2-3 days before learning a task had a harder time with recall (Fekete 1985). This means that their daily sleep/wake schedule was shifted a few hours off from their normal (to a new phase) and that impacted their learning. A study in humans confirmed this, showing that students who shifted their sleep-wake cycle by 2 hours had difficulty concentrating, even if they were sleeping a full 8 hours nightly (Taub 1974). These findings suggest that the usual student model of staying up late to study and dramatically changing sleep schedules the days preceding an exam to cram might be hindering students’ ability to learn information and recall that information on tests. More importantly, when the schedules of model organisms are altered to mimic circadian disruption, there are impacts on the hippocampus: a part of the brain that is crucial for learning and memory.
The hippocampus is associated with spatial memory and the crucial process of forming long-term memories. Neurogenesis—the growth of new neurons—in the hippocampus is associated with learning (Epp 2007) and it has been shown that circadian disruption can inhibit neurogenesis through many pathways. We will only briefly discuss three major ones here. First, a growth factor in the brain known as BDNF will increase the birth of neurons (Taliaz 2010), and disruption to sleep and circadian rhythms alters the fluctuations of BDNF (Guzman-Marin 2006). Second, melatonin (that hormone that makes you sleepy at night) promotes neurogenesis and helps new, growing neurons stay alive (Ramirez-Rodriguez 2009). Melatonin has also been implicated in long-term potentiation, which is the process by which your brain forms long-term memories (El-Sherif et al, 2003). Melatonin levels are decreased by lights at night, and by that late night coffee that students survive on (Wright 1997). What’s more, circadian disruption has been shown to disturb the normal fluctuations of cortisol (Wotus et al 2013). Disruption of cortisol rhythms has been associated with structural changes in the hippocampus (Gartside et al 2003) and deficits in certain types of training of rats and mice. All three of these pathways lie at the center of sleep, body clocks, and learning and memory.
Overall, there are multiple pathways–including BDNF, melatonin, and cortisol—by which circadian disruption can inhibit one of the main learning centers in our brain, the hippocampus. All of these findings suggest there is strong biological evidence in favor of students calling it a night, even when an exam is coming up. If anything is to be learned from these animal models, humans should study during the day, and should get plenty of sleep on a regular basis so that their daily learning is not inhibited. Maintaining your body’s schedule seems to be especially important leading up to the time of the test, as circadian disruption surrounding learning inhibits retention and recall– this means that all-nighter will probably just leave you tired and without much information retained. So swap your late-night coffee for a glass of water, and grab a blanket instead of your books, and spend the night allowing your brain to condense the information you’ve already obtained and prepare for another day of learning.

Much of this blog post was adapted from the content and references of the review paper:
A Time to Remember: The Role of Circadian Clocks in Learning and Memory
Benjamin L. Smarr, Kimberly J. Jennings, Joseph R. Driscoll, Lance J. Kriegsfield.
Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014 June; 128(3): 283-303


References specifically mentioned in blog:
1. Decker S, McConnaughey S, Page TL. Circadian regulation of insect olfactory learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2007;104(40):15905–15910. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0702082104
2. Hauber W, Bareiss A. Facilitative effects of an adenosine A1/A2 receptor blockade on spatial memory performance of rats: selective enhancement of reference memory retention during the light period. Behavioral brain research. 2001;118(1):43–52.
3. Hoffmann H, Balschun D. Circadian differences in maze performance of c57bl/6 ola mice. 1992 Aug
4. El-Sherif Y, Tesoriero J, Hogan MV, Wieraszko A. Melatonin regulates neuronal plasticity in the hippocampus. Journal of neuroscience research. 2003;72(4):454–460. doi: 10.1002/jnr.10605
5. Epp JR, Spritzer MD, Galea LAM. Hippocampus-dependent learning promotes survival of new neurons in the dentate gyrus at a specific time during cell maturation. Neuroscience.2007;149(2):273–285. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.07.046.
6. Fekete M, van Ree JM, Niesink RJ, de Wied D. Disrupting circadian rhythms in rats induces retrograde amnesia. Physiology & behavior. 1985;34(6):883–887
7. Gartside SE, Leitch MM, McQuade R, Swarbrick DJ. Flattening the glucocorticoid rhythm causes changes in hippocampal expression of messenger RNAs coding structural and functional proteins: implications for aging and depression. Neuropsychopharmacology: official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28(5):821–829. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300104
8. Guzman-Marin R, Ying Z, Suntsova N, Methippara M, Bashir T, Szymusiak R, McGinty D. Suppression of hippocampal plasticity-related gene expression by sleep deprivation in rats. The Journal of physiology. 2006;575(Pt 3):807–819. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.115287.
9. Ramírez-Rodríguez G, Klempin F, Babu H, Benítez-King G, Kempermann G. Melatonin modulates cell survival of new neurons in the hippocampus of adult mice. Neuropsychopharmacology: official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34(9):2180–2191. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.46.
10. Taliaz D, Stall N, Dar DE, Zangen A. Knockdown of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in specific brain sites precipitates behaviors associated with depression and reduces neurogenesis. Molecular psychiatry. 2010;15(1):80–92. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.67.
11. Taub JM, Berger RJ. Acute shifts in the sleep-wakefulness cycle: Effects on performance and mood. Psychosom Med. l974;36: 1661 73.
12. Thacher, Pamela V. University Students and the “All Nighter”: Correlates and Patterns of Students’ Engagement in a Single Night of Total Sleep Deprivation, Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 2008: 6:1, 16-31, DOI: 10.1080/15402000701796114
13. Wotus C, Lilley TR, Neal AS, Suleiman NL, Schmuck SC, Smarr BL, de la Iglesia HO. Forced desynchrony reveals independent contributions of suprachiasmatic oscillators to the daily plasma corticosterone rhythm in male rats. PloS one. 2013;8(7):e68793. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068793
14. Wright, KP Jr., et al. Caffeine and light effects on nighttime melatonin and temperature levels in sleep-deprived humans. Brain Research. 1997 Jan 30;747(1):78-84.

Latest updates news (2024-07-26 22:32):

genf20 plus g5d before and after | male enhancement that is natural if viagra rO6 is not effective | does enalapril affect aLm erectile dysfunction | what is in usQ viagra that makes it work | cysteine most effective erectile dysfunction | how long does it take for viagra to DjY react | how to turn off oDb your sex drive | free shipping is nofap bad | otc sleeoing pills MFp erectile dysfunction | men with 3MD huge loads | urinary health tips LLN for men | fenugreek OHe pills near me | PbL bathmate hercules vs x30 | vitamin e for penile growth 3tY | melanotan ii r6X erectile dysfunction | viagra online sale age | what does real viagra P9c look like | erectile Pnd dysfunction ayurvedic tablets | whats better girth bYN or length | cigarette smoking 7yG causes erectile dysfunction | cmv hhv big sale | erectile dysfunction falls under kFP which speciality | i got online shop pills | gcY erectile dysfunction and enlarged left ventricle | 9lU erectile dysfunction and steroids | stay hard pills at 2ay gnc | tumblr penis growth official | manhood rx reviews free shipping | how LOO to please a woman sexually | UOa vitacost male enhancement with maca | 15 day gold XhP male enhancement pills | what can you use instead of viagra Q1x | can you xbo get free viagra | does cocaine make 3ss you impotent | recommended testosterone booster free trial | vitamin e sex drive bOJ | OUG does priamax male enhancement works | sildamax low price | best men sexual enhancer tGd | weak pelvic xno floor muscles erectile dysfunction | average flacid yY5 penis length | age prescription mP6 erectile dysfunction | nature bound male enhancement jrX | for sale my penis bigger | testosterone and enhancement booster bBk | how safe is male enhancement for EEx high blood pressure | wild sex pill online sale | how to naturally Vvj enlarge your penus | erectile dysfunction sQf home remedy india | severe erectile sVM dysfunction causes